February 10, 2010

Bioe 109

Winter 2010

Lecture 15

Sexual selection

- what is sexual selection?

- Darwin defined sexual selection as:

the fitness advantage that some individuals have over others of the same sex solely with respect to reproduction.

- Darwin viewed sexual selection as being quite distinct from natural selection.

- to him, natural selection favored variations among individuals based mainly on survivorship.

- characters such as the peacock’s tail posed a problem for Darwin’s theory because they appeared to incur a cost to survival - surely a mutant peacock with a tail 1/10 the length would be more efficient at escaping predators.

- Darwin came up with a solution to this problem by proposing that characters may evolve that conferred a disadvantage to survivorship if they conferred an advantage in reproductive success.

- although Darwin made an important distinction between natural and sexual selection, there is really little need for this.

- sexual selection simply addresses one fitness component: reproductive success.

- a character that increases reproductive success acts to increase overall fitness - even if it has a detrimental effect on another fitness component such as survivorship.

Total Fitness = Viability + Fecundity + Longevity + Mating success

- Darwin recognized that “trade-offs” may occur between fitness components.

- thus, a trait increasing mating success may evolve even if it causes a reduction in viability as long as there is an overall increase in fitness.

Why does sexual selection occur?

- because of an “asymmetry” of sex - i.e., sexual reproduction creates different selection pressures for males and females.

- the basic argument is rather simple; in a large number of species, females invest more in their offspring than do males.

- eggs are more costly to produce than sperm and the total reproductive output of females is potentially much lower than males.

- since eggs (and pregnancies) are more expensive to produce than sperm, an important difference emerges in what limits the lifetime reproductive success of a female versus a male.

females:limited by # of eggs and/or pregnancies

males:limited by # females mated

- in other words, we predict that access to females will be a limiting resource for males (usually) but access to males will not be a limiting resource for females (usually).

- this sets up a conflict that is manifest as sexual selection.

- the difference between males and females leads, in very general terms, to two main consequences:

1. males should be competitive - they should compete among themselves for access to females.

2. females should be choosy - since her investment is larger than males, she has more to lose by making a bad decision.

- these are the two forms of sexual selection identified by Darwin: “male-male competition” and “female choice”.

- male-male competition is also called “intrasexual selection” because selection occurs among males.

- female choice is also called “intersexual selection” because selection involves an interaction between males and females.

- this was first shown in a classic experiment by Bateman in 1948.

- Bateman carried out a very simple experiment to examine the reproductive success of male and female Drosophila melanogaster.

- he placed 3 male and 3 female fruitflies in bottles and allowed them to mate.

- all flies used in these experiments carried unique sets of genetic markers.

- by examining the resulting progeny from these cultures, Bateman was able to see who mated with whom and how many offspring each individual produced.

- a clear difference emerged between males and females.

- for males, reproductive success increased in direct proportion to the number of mates.

- for females, reproductive success did not increase with more than one mate.

- furthermore, variation in the number of mates among males was higher than seen among females.

- some males had no mates (the biggest losers), the biggest winners were those that had 3.

- for females, the vast majority had one or two mates - none had 0 and virtually none had 3.

- sexual selection, variation in fitness due to variation in success at getting mates, was much stronger in males than in females.

- similar results have been obtained in other species – like the rough-skinned newt example described in the textbook.

- in situations where males are limiting resources to females (such as in pipefish), things are reversed (as theory predicts).

- here, females are larger and more brightly colored than males (who have specialized brood pouches they use to incubate eggs laid by females).

- females are aggressive and fight amongst themselves for access to males.

- in pipefish, females experience a wider variance in reproductive success than males and gain a linear increase in fitness with an increasing number of mates.

Intrasexual selection

- intrasexual selection typically occurs in species where individual males can monopolize access to females.

- this monopolization can take the form of some direct control of the females themselves or through the control of some resource important to females.

- male-male competition can take on a number of distinct forms.

1. Combat

- outright combat is the most obvious form of male-male conflict.

- combat among males can lead to sexual dimorphism in size in which males are larger than females

- it can also lead to the evolution of weaponry, such as horns in ungulates and beetles.

2. Sperm competition

- male-male competition does not stop when copulation is over.

- the true determinant of a male’s mating success is not whether he copulates, but whether his sperm actually fertilize eggs.

- if a female mates with multiple males within a short period of time, there will be a race among the males to successfully fertilize her eggs.

- females from a wide variety of many species are known to produce offspring that are sired by more than one male.

- multiple paternity clearly sets the stage for sperm competition.

- what traits would be selected in sperm competition?

- one obvious one would be to simply increase the amount of ejaculate, thus increasing one male’s chances of displacing sperm of other males.

- other less direct forms of sperm competition involve the transfer of compounds into the females reproductive tract that modify her behavior.

- notable here are seminal fluid proteins.

- in species like Drosophila and other insects these proteins have been found to have a strong influence on many important traits of females including longevity, egg-laying rate and re-mating rate.

3. Infanticide

- in some species of mammals competition among males continues even beyond conception.

- one of the most striking examples of infanticide involves the African lion.

- the basic social unit of lions is called the pride.

- prides consist of a group of closely related females (mothers, daughters, sisters, nieces, etc.)

- the pride also consists of a small group of resident males - two or three is the usual number.

- the males are usually related to each other - we know this from DNA fingerprinting work - and not related to the females.

- this situation arises because females reaching sexual maturity remain in the pride where they were born whereas males disperse to other prides.

- males must fight and defeat the group of resident males in order to gain control of the pride.

- the average time that male lions maintain control of a pride is only about 2 years.

- because residence in the pride is the key to reproductive success of male lions, they quickly begin trying to father cubs.

- one impediment to quick fatherhood is the presence of still-nursing cubs, because females do not return to breeding condition until their cubs are weaned.

- how do male lions overcome this problem?

- they resort to the grisly task of killing any cubs in the pride that are not weaned.

- this is a highly successful strategy.

- it results in females returning to breeding condition an average of 8 months earlier than they otherwise would.

- however, it has a heavy cost on the species.

- about 25% of all cub mortality in the first year is caused by infanticide and about 10% of overall lion mortality.

4. Alternative male reproductive strategies.

- alternative male reproductive strategies are also very common.

- an excellent example is provided by the phenomenon of “jacking” in Pacific salmon.

- in some species (coho and sockeye), a proportion of males spend a small time at sea (6 months) and mature at a very small size.

- they then attempt to “sneak” fertilizations from pairs of large, sea-run fish by rushing in and releasing sperm at the appropriate time.

- these sneaky males may enjoy a substantial reproductive success.

- this strategy exhibits some degree of genetic control but also is influenced by environmental factors (notably the hatchery environment which can generate a much higher proportion of jacks than seen in nature).

Intersexual selection

- in species where males advertise for mates and females choose among them, inter-sexual selection can lead to the evolution of elaborate coloration, exaggerated male secondary characteristics and courtship behavior.

- male-male competition was traditionally believed to be the prevalent type of sexual selection.

- now, female choice is recognized as being equally important and there is much recent experimental evidence to support it.

- however, a critical question needs to be answered:

Why should females have preferences for certain male characters?

- some types of female choice are easy to explain - for example, in many species of birds and spiders the male will court the female by bringing her food items.

- in the common tern, females show a strong preference for males that bring a lot of food.

- the amount of food a male brings during courtship turns out to be strongly correlated with the amount of food he brings to feed chicks.

- the female clearly has her reasons for selecting males with this character.

- this is an example of direct benefits.

- how about lekking species?

- leks are aggregations of males that gather in sites, defend small territories, and display for females - e.g., grouse, ruffs, Jackson’s widowbird.

- in lekking species, a male’s only contribution is his sperm - how does female choice evolve here?

- let us consider four models.

1. Runaway selection

- the first theoretical answer to the evolution of why females prefer certain types of males was first developed by R.A. Fisher in a process he called “runaway selection”.

- in this process a genetic correlation develops between a male trait and female preference for that trait.

- the simplest model assumes two genetic loci - one determining female preference (P locus) for the exaggerated male trait and one controlling the male trait itself (T locus).

- let’s use the example of stalkeye flies in which females appear to mate with males having long eyestalks.

- females having an allele (P1) that causes them to prefer males with long eyestalks will produce sons possessing that trait (T1).

- these males will have an advantage over other males in the population because some females prefer them.

- the sisters of these males with longer eyestalks will have a preference for this trait because their mother did.

- the correlation will be strengthened by linkage disequilibrium between the two loci.

- now, consider a mutation that causes a slightly longer eyestalk (T2).

- because the female preference allele (P1) is tightly linked to this locus, preference for the exaggerated character is automatic.

- each generation, males may develop slightly longer eyestalks (by the action of female choice) but each generation females prefer longer eyestalks than their mothers did.

- this results in a positive feedback loop that can result in “runaway selection”.

- eventually the selection for even greater male traits will be opposed by natural selection when the trait reduces male survivorship.

2. Good genes models

- the good genes hypothesis predicts that females choose males with certain traits because the trait is an honest “indicator” of overall genetic quality.

- any male trait that is correlated with overall genetic quality is thus an indicator of “good genes”.

- the trait thus serves as a “guide” by which females choose mates.

- is there support for the good genes model?

- many studies have obtained support for the good genes model.

- one of the best examples is that described in the textbook involving gray tree frogs by Allison Welch.

- in this species, males differ in the length of their mating calls and females appear to prefer males with longer calls.

- the good genes hypothesis predicts that the progeny of longer-calling males should be genetically superior to shorter-calling males.

- females are thus using the call length as an indicator trait and it behaves as an honest signal.

- Welch et al. (1998) did a nice experiment in which they fertilized the eggs of females with two males - a “short caller” and a “long-caller”.

- having the same mother but different fathers, the progeny are half-sibs.

- the tadpoles were reared under two feeding regimens - low and high food.

- they then examined a number of fitness related traits including larval growth rates, time to metamorphosis, mass at metamorphosis, larval survival, and juvenile growth.

- out of 18 individual comparisons, the progeny of short calling males never did better than long callers.

- long callers did significantly better in 8 comparisons.

- the difference between males is statistically significant.

- thus, we can conclude that females are benefiting by choosing longer-calling males.

3. Sensory bias

- sensory bias models predict that female preference for certain male traits may evolve prior to the appearance of the male trait itself.

- in other words, females have a pre-existing sensory bias for the trait.

- females will thus prefer males that happen by chance to produce that trait.

- under this model, the attraction that the female has for the male trait is an incidental by-product of her sensory system.

- there is no direct selection on the female to be attracted to that male trait.

- evidence supporting the sensory bias model comes from phylogenetic studies.

- for example, in wolf spiders female preference for tufted forelimbs appears in a phylogeny of the group prior to the appearance of the male trait itself.

- this suggests that the tufts evolved to take advantage of a pre-existing bias in female to prefer males with these tufts.

- a number of additional studies have demonstrated pre-existing female preferences for male traits that don’t yet exist.

- examples include, swordtails (the male sword), two species of grassfinches (crests of feathers on the head), and tungara frogs (addition of chucks to mating calls).

- sensory bias models differ dramatically from “good genes” models because females are not assumed to obtain any benefits whatsoever from the male trait.

- the male is modifying female behavior and stimulating her to mate but she gains no direct or indirect benefit in choosing that male over any other.

4 Chase-away selection

- this model is based on the prediction that the optimal mating rate of males is not the same as females.

- therefore, males are always attempting to stimulate females to mate at a level that benefits themselves but not the female.

- this leads to an intraspecific Red Queen process.

- males evolve adaptations (i.e., ornaments, courtship displays) that enable them to mate females at an elevated rate.

- females, in turn, evolve counter-adaptations that enable them to resist the male adaptation and mate at more optimal levels.

- this leads to a perpetual cycle of adaptations and counter-adaptations involving a class of “sexually-antagonistic genes”.

- evidence favoring the chase-away model has been obtained in Drosophila in which females were prevented from “counter-adapting” to male-benefit genes.

- in about 32 generations, adaptations appeared in the males that allowed them to increase their fitness at the expense of females.