Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery (1999) 1, 181–191

Recommendations for the housing of cats in the home, in catteries and animal shelters, in laboratories and in veterinary surgeries

I Rochlitz

Animal Welfare and

Human Animal Interactions Group, Department of Clinical Veterinary Medicine, University of Cambridge, Madingley Road, Cambridge CB3 OES, UK

In recent years, there has been an increase in interest in applied ethology and animal welfare, and an increase in the popularity of the domestic cat. This has stimulated research on the behaviour and welfare of cats kept in different environments. This article presents a review of the recent research and makes recommendations for the housing of domestic cats in the home, in catteries and animal shelters, in laboratories and in veterinary surgeries.

The popularity of the domestic cat has increased steadily in the past few years. In 1998, the owned cat population in the UK was 8 million and exceeded the pet dog population by 1.1 million (Pet Food Manufacturers' Association 1999). Over 20% (5.1 million) of British households owned at least one cat and 37% of these households had more than one cat.

In recent years, research has been carried out on the behaviour and welfare of cats kept in different environments. These include laboratories (Podberscek et al 1991, McCune 1992, van den Bos & de Cock Buning 1994b, van den Bos 1998), animal shelters (Durman 1991, McCune 1992, Roy 1992, Smith et al 1994, Kessler & Turner 1997, Rochlitz 1997), quarantine and boarding catteries (Kessler & Turner 1997, Rochlitz et al 1998b) and the home (Bernstein & Strack 1996). This paper reviews the recent research, and presents guidelines on the housing requirements of cats kept in the home, in catteries and animal shelters, in laboratories and in veterinary surgeries.

Assessment of welfare

The domestic cat has evolved from a carnivore with an essentially solitary lifestyle where, in many contexts, there is no need for large, exaggerated or ritualised signals to develop. Assessment of welfare may initially seem difficult, as cats do not have as wide a behavioural repertoire for visual communication (eg posture, facial expression, tail position) as, for example, the highly social, group-living dog. However, the UK Cat Behaviour Working Group (1995) has published an ethogram (a catalogue of discrete, species-typical behaviour patterns that form the basic behavioural repertoire of the species) for behavioural studies of the domestic cat, and stress scores, based on behaviour have been developed (McCune 1992, Kessler & Turner 1997). Physiological measures such as urinary cortisol have also been used to assess welfare (Carlstead et al 1992, Carlstead et al 1993, Rochlitz et al 1998b).

Cats are more likely to respond to poor environmental conditions by becoming inactive and by inhibiting normal behaviours such as self-maintenance (feeding, grooming and elimination), exploration or play, than by actively showing abnormal behaviour (McCune 1992, Rochlitz 1997). Cats with illness may also modify their behaviour in a similar way. Keeping cats in an enriched, stimulating environment that encourages a wide range of normal behaviours will not only enhance their welfare, but also

General recommendations

The main points to be considered when designing or evaluating housing for cats are listed below. The particular requirements of cats kept in specific environments (the home, catteries and animal shelters, laboratories and veterinary surgeries) are considered in the next section. When cats are housed together the control of infectious disease is of primary importance, but this aspect will not be included in this review.

'Size of enclosure(The terms enclosure, pen and cage are used interchangeably)

Within an enclosure (the internal environment), there should be adequate separation between feeding, resting and elimination (litter tray) areas. The enclosure should be large enough to allow cats to express a range of normal behaviours, and to permit the caretaker or owner to carry out cleaning procedures easily.

When cats are housed in groups, there should also be enough space for cats to keep themselves separate from others (Fig 1). Conflict-regulating mechanisms are important to maintain stability of groups in some species (van den Bos 1998) but group-living cats lack distinct dominant, hierarchies and post-conflict mechanisms such reconciliation (van den Bos & de Cock Bunir 1994a, van den Bos 1998). They are not adapt (to living in close proximity to each other and reduce the likelihood of aggression by establishing distances between themselves (Leyhausen 1979).

If an enclosure is too small, there may be an increase in agonistic encounters or cats will attempt to avoid each other decreasing their activity (Leyhausen 1979,van den Bos & de Cock Bunir 1994a, van den Bos 1998).

The vertical dimension is particularly important as regards the provision of appropriate internal complexity (see next section), so cages show be of adequate height.

Complexity of enclosure

Beyond a certain minimum size, it is the quality rather than the quantity of space that is important. Most cats are active, have the ability to clip well and are well-adapted for concealment (Eisenberg 1989). They use elevated areasasvantage points from which to monitor their surroundings(DeLuca & Kranda 1992, Holmes 1993, James 1995). Enclosures should contain structures that make maximal use of the verticaldimension, such as shelves, climbing frames, platforms, hammocks and raised walkways placed at various heights (Fig 2).

As cats are more likely to rest alone than with others, there should be a sufficient number of resting areas for all cats in the enclosure. (Podberscek et al 1991, Bernstein & Strack 1996). Hiding is a behaviour that cats often show in response to stimuli or changes in their environment, and to avoid interactions with other cats or people (McCune 1992, Carlstead et al 1993, James 1995, Rochlitz et al 1998b). Resting areas where cats can retreat to and be concealed, in addition to 'open' resting areas (eg. shelves), are essential for their well-being (Fig 3). Visual barriers, such as vertical panels, can also be used to enable cats to get away from others and hide. If it is necessary to observe the cat closely, a box open on two or three sides, or a deep-sided tray, can be used.Rest areas should have comfortable bedding, which will reduce the likelihood of cats resting in their litter trays (DeLuca & Kranda 1992). Tests indicate that cats prefer polyester fleece to cotton-looped towel, woven rush-matting and corrugated cardboard for lying on (Hawthorne et al 1995).

There should be a sufficient number of litter trays, at least one per two cats (Hoskins 1996), sited away from feeding and resting areas. Cats can have individual preferences for litter characteristics, so it may be necessary to provide a range of litter types and designs of litter trays. Surfaces for claw abrasion (e.g. scratch posts, rush matting, carpet, wood) should also be avail-able, as well as toys (Fig 2). Objects which move, have complex textures and mimic prey characteristics are the most successful at promoting play (Hall & Bradshaw 1998). A variety of toys should be available, and novelty is also important so toys should be replaced regularly. Most cats play alone rather than in groups (Podberscek et al. 1991), so the cage should be large enough to permit them to play without disturbing other cats. Consideration should also be given to providing containers of grass, which some cats like to chew and may be important for the elimination of furballs (trichobezoars). Catnip (Nepeta cataria) may also be provided, either as a dried herb or contained in toys.

Another environmental enrichment technique is to increase the time animals spend in predatory and feeding behaviour. McCune (1995) suggests putting dry food into containers 'With holes through which the cat has to extract individual pieces.

Quality of the external environment

The environment around the enclosure (the external environment) will have an impact on the cat's welfare. Efforts should be made to increase olfactory, visual and auditory stimulation, for example by creating enclosures that look out on to areas of human and animal activity, or by providing access to an outdoor run. A technique used in some animal houses is the playing of a radio, to provide music and human conversation (Benn 1995, James 1995, Newberry 1995). This is thought to prevent animals from being startled by sudden noises and habituate them to human voices, and to provide a degree of continuity in the environment (James 1995).

Contact with conspecifics

The cat is a social carnivore that regularly inter-acts with conspecifics (Leyhausen 1979, Voith & Borchelt 1986, Sandell 1989). Most cats can be • housed in groups providing that they are well socialised to other cats, and that there is sufficient space, easy access to feeding and elimination areas and a sufficient number of concealed retreats and resting places. When cats are kept in large groups, it may be necessary to distribute feed, rest and elimination areas in a number of different sites, to prevent certain cats from monopolising one area and denying others access (van den Bos & de Cock Buning 1994b).

Many factors will determine the ideal group size, but it seems that 20 to 25 individuals is the maximal number for cats in laboratories (James 1995, Hubrecht & Turner 1998). In animal shelters, where infectious disease is a frequent problem and there is a regular turnover of cats, they should be kept in smaller groups. Cats that fail to adapt satisfactorily to group-living should be identified and housed singly.

Contact with humans and quality of care

In addition to interacting with conspecifics, domestic cats interact frequently and effectively with humans (Voith & Borchelt 1986, Turner 1995). While cleaning and feeding times provide

some opportunities for interactions, a period of time which is not part of routine caretaking procedures should be set aside every day for cats to interact with caretakers or owners (Fig 4). Some cats may prefer to be petted and handled, while others may prefer to interact via a toy (Karsh & Turner 1988).

Owners and caretakers need to be knowledgeable about the behaviour of the animals they are responsible for, since behavioural changes are often the first indicators of illness or other causes of poor welfare. A study of owners who relinquished pets to shelters found that many lacked knowledge about the normal behaviour of their pets (Salman et al 1998). Owners are more likely to develop a successful relationship with their p@t if they have realistic expectations about its behaviour and requirements (Kidd et al 1992). Formal training in animal husbandry should be mandatory for those involved in the day to day care of cats in shelters, catteries, laboratories and veterinary surgeries, and owners should be encouraged to obtain information about cats from a variety of sources.

The sociability of cats to people and to con-specifics is influenced by many factors, such as the handling of kittens at a young age, their exposure to other cats and people, and genetic influences (McCune et al 1995). Attention to management procedures and selective breeding will ensure that cats are well socialised.

Specific housing environments

The following section describes the particular requirements of cats in the home, in catteries and animal shelters, in laboratories and in veterinary surgeries. The aim should be to provide good housing conditions, regardless of the length of the housing period. Whether it will be housed for 2 days, 2 weeks, 2 months or 2 years is of no relevance to the animal; its well-being is determined by the conditions it lives in day to day.

Clearly, in some situations compromises or modifications may have to be made. For example, in quarantine catteries or animal shelters where cats may be confined for long periods, it will be particularly important to ensure that the enclosure and surroundings are complex and stimulating, and that the cats receive sufficient human contact. In veterinary surgeries (where the cat may be ill), or in laboratories (where experimental procedures are carried out), it may be necessary to restrict the cat's movement and to monitor it closely. Nevertheless, just because the animal will be housed for a short period of time is not an excuse for providing poor housing conditions.

The home

In the United States, between 50 and 60% of pet cats are housed indoors (Luke 1996, Patronek et al 1997); this figure is lower for cats in Britain although data have not been published. Some; authors (Landsberg 1996, Miller 1996) feel that cats are best housed indoors, while others believe that the cat's quality of life is enhanced if it is .Allowed outdoors (O'Farrell & Neville 1994, Hubrecht & Turner 1998). Cats that roam freely outdoors are more likely to be exposed to infectious disease, involved in agonistic encounters with cats and other animals, injured or killed by motor vehicles, and may go missing. Another stated argument for keeping cats indoors is to protect wildlife populations from predation (Tatronek 1998). Cats with outdoor access can probably compensate to some degree for unsatisfactory conditions in their home environment (Turner 1995). Some behaviourists note-that cats kept exclusively indoors are over-represented in the population referred to them with behavioural problems (Hubrecht & Turner 1998). Providing secure enclosures within a garden, or leash-training a cat, are solutions that enable the cat to benefit from outdoor access without undue risk. Whether cats are confined indoors or allowed outdoors, housing conditions in the home should follow the recommendations listed previously.

Bernstein and Strack (1996) described the use of space and patterns of interaction of 14 unrelated, neutered domestic cats, who lived together in a single-storey house at a density of one cat per 10 m2. The cats did not have access to the outdoors. Most of the cats had favourite spots within the rooms that they used. Some individuals had their own unique place, but more commonly several cats chose the same favourite spot. These areas were shared either physically, by cats occupying the space together or, more often, temporally by cats occupying them at different times of the day. There was very little aggression and no fighting between the cats. Individuals seemed to peacefully co-exist by avoiding each other for most of the time. Female cats are said to be more suited to an indoor existence than male cats, because feral males have a bigger home range than feral females (Mertens & Schar 1988). Bernstein and Strack (1996) found that neutered males had an average home range of four to five rooms (out of 10), and neutered females a range of three to 3.6 rooms. It is likely that both neutered males and neutered females can be successfully housed indoors, providing there is sufficient quantity and quality of space.

Boarding and quarantine catteries and animal shelters

In 1995, the Chartered Institute of Environmental Health published model licence conditions and guidance for cat boarding establishments in the UK (CIEH Animal Boarding Establishments Working Party 1995). These guidelines serve as a basis upon which local environmental health officers issue licences to boarding catteries. Concerns have been expressed regarding the welfare of animals in quarantine (Bennett 1997, Rochlitz et al 1998a,b) but currently there exists only a voluntary code of practice (Ministry of Agriculture 1995). However, legislation on welfare standards in quarantine premises is likely to be introduced in the future. Animal shelters in the UK are not subject to legislation on standards of animal care, although the Feline Advisory Bureau has recently published a cat rescue manual (Haughie 1998), which includes a code of practice for cat rescue facilities.

Durman (1991) described behavioural indicators of stress in cats who were introduced into communal pens at an animal shelter. Major behavioural changes occurred in the first 4 days, but changes continued throughout the first month as the cats adapted. Kessler and Turner (1997) studied 140 cats entering a boarding cattery, and monitored their adaptation using a Cat-Stress-Score. Stress levels decreased during the observation period, with a pronounced reduction in the first 4 days, and two-thirds of cats adjusted satisfactorily within 2 weeks. In another shelter study (Rochlitz 1997), behavioural and physiological data indicated that cats showed signs of adaptation within 2 weeks. However, cats took longer (5 weeks) to-adjust to conditions in a quarantine cattery (Rochlitz et al 1998b).

Smith et al (1994) examined the behaviour of cats in shelters, with specific reference to their spatial distribution. Cats used structures more often than the floor of the pens, and high structures, which provided vantage points, were used more frequently than low ones. These findings were confirmed in other studies (Durman 1991, Podberscek et al 1991, Roy 1992, Rochlitz et al 1998b). Roy (1992) found that cats preferred wood as a substrate to plastic, and also liked materials that maintain a constant temperature such as straw, hay, wood shavings and fabric. In an animal shelter, the presence of a toy within an enclosure may make the animal more attractive to prospective owners (Wells & Hepper 1992).

Techniques have been developed for identifying cats that are unfriendly towards other cats or people (Kessler 1997). Cats that are poorly socialised towards conspecifics should be housed singly, and cats that are poorly socialised towards people, such as feral cats, should not initially be subjected to a high demand for inter-action with shelter staff and visitors (Kessler 1997). However, cats may become more socialised towards humans and subsequently make more rewarding pets, if they receive regular socialisation sessions.

Hoskins (1995) examined the effect of human contact on the reactions of cats in a rescue shelter. Cats that had received additional handling sessions, where they interacted closely with a familiar person, could subsequently be held for longer by an unfamiliar person than cats who had not received additional handling sessions.

Concern has been expressed regarding the potentially harmful effects long periods of housing in a shelter may have on the animal's behaviour (Wells & Hepper 1992) and the likelihood that the animal will be able to integrate successfully into a home environment after adoption. In a study of cats adopted from a shelter, there was no correlation between the length of time the cat had been in the shelter (the longest time was 9.5 months) and the time it took to adapt to its new home (Rochlitz et al 1996). Another study found that being in a quarantine cattery for six months caused changes in the temperament and behaviour of cats. and in the relationship between cats and their owners (Rochlitz et al 1998a).