ADHD: Introduction and Historical Overview
ADHD is a common neurodevelopmental disorder with a prevalence of 3 to 5 per cent in the elementary school population. It is one of the most common disorders of childhood, accounting for a large number of referrals to child mental health facilities.
While views of what is now called ADHD have evolved over the years, the essential characteristics of this disorder have been recognized for almost a century.
Since this time, the disorder has been referred to by a variety of labels, various characteristics have been highlighted as the essential feature of the disorder, and a number of factors have been highlighted as causal in the development of the disorder.
In this session, we will briefly overview some of the historical milestones and trends in the evolution of this disorder.
EARLY DESCRIPTIONS
Still (1902 described 43 children seen in medical practice who displayed an number of features we now associate with ADHD.
CHARACTERISTICS:
- Children were aggressive, defiant, resistant to discipline, overly emotional and showed "little inhibitory volition".
- Need for immediate gratification was characteristic of most.
- Children displayed major problems with sustained attention
- The majority were overly active and they tended to be prone to accidental injury.
- Some were described as having a "major defect in moral control" and showed an insensitivity to punishment.
- Many displayed minor physical anomalies (large head size, malformed palate, and epicanthal folds) which he referred to as "stigmata of degeneration".
- Some, but not all, had a history of convulsions or documented brain damage.
- Some had tic disorders.
- Family histories of alcoholism, criminality and affective disorders were relatively common.
As a group, most of the children Still described developed these problems before age 8; There was a 3 to 1 sex ratio with more males than females being represented.
Still suggested that the problems in sustained attention and deficits in inhibiting behavior and "moral control" were related and were manifestations of underlying neurological deficiency.
He speculated that these children either had a decreased threshold for inhibition of responding to stimuli or a "cortical disconnection syndrome", "where intellect is disassociated from will" that could be due to some sort of "neuronal cell modification".
Discussion Question:
To what extent was Still's description of these children consistent with what we know about ADHD today?
- Problems of attention, activity and impulsivity.
- Presence of minor physical anomalies.
- Likely neurological involvement.
- Association with tic disorders, oppositional behavior and conduct problems.
- Sex ratio, early age of onset.
In sum, Still's group were similar to children that might be seen as displaying ADHD today, particularly if ADHD children with comorbid conditions were considered.
In addition to Still's early account, others such as Tredgold (1908) also described children who displayed many of the features that are sometimes associated with ADHD today.
EARLY SUGGESTIONS OF BIIOLOGICAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO THE DISORDER
THE ENCEPHALITIS EPIDEMIC OF 1917 - 1918
Many children who were affected by this disease and died.
Others who survived the acute stages of this illness experience major cognitive and behavioral sequelae, including characteristics such as hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattentive and socially disruptive behavior.
Often had memory difficulties as well as other types of cognitive impairments.
Many also displayed features that we would now think of as reflecting oppositional defiant or conduct disordered behavior.
As these characteristics were observed in children who were documented to have neurological disease, it provided early evidence for the notion that behavioral problems of childhood (like those we now associate with ADHD can result f rom neurological impairment.
OTHER EARLY LINKS BETWEEN BRAIN INSULT AND BEHAVIORAL DIFFICULTIES (1930's and 1940's)
Many investigators had an interest in the relationship between "behavioral pathology" and "brain disease."
During this time a range of cognitive and behavioral impairments such as mental retardation, learning problems, and problems with restlessness, hyperactivity and impulsiveness were found to be associated with a childhood history of birth trauma, head injury, viral infections (such as rubella), and exposure to certain types of toxins (e.g., lead).
These findings provided further reason to believe that there might be an association between the disorder we now call ADHD and some sort of brain damage or dysfunction.
FRONTAL LOBE ABLATION STUDIES WITH PRIMATES
Early interest in the possible link between attention deficits, hyperactivity and brain impairment was also sparked by the observed similarity between the behavior of hyperactive children and the behavioral effects resulting from frontal lobe lesions in primates.
In the 1930's Frontal Lobe Ablation Studies of Monkeys suggested that frontal lobe lesions often result in excessive restlessness, poor ability to sustain interest in activities, and disorganization in behavior.
This resulted in early speculation that hyperactivity in children might result from pathological defects in the area of the frontal lobes.
THE CONCEPT OF MINIMAL BRAIN DYSFUNCTION
During the late 1940's and 50's it was commonly assumed that the problems displayed by children like those considered here were the result of some sort of neurological impairment or brain injury.
Indeed, there was evidence, by this time, that the development of behavioral problems of activity level, attention, impulsivity, and conduct (along with others), CAN result from neurological difficulties.
There was no real problem in assuming that behavioral problems, like the ones considered here, may be the result of brain damage in cases where there is a clear history of trauma or illness that could cause in neurological impairment.
However, some working in this area took things a step further, by making the assumption that behaviors known to result from brain damage can serve as indicators of brain damage where the cause of the child's behavior problems have not been documented.
Evolution of the MBD Concept
In a series of studies, in the 1940's and 1950's, Alfred Strauss and his colleagues attempted to isolate characteristics that would discriminate between groups of mentally retarded children with and without a history of documented brain damage.
These studies suggested a number of psychological and behavioral markers to be reliably associated with a history of brain damage, including:
- Hyperactivity
- Aggressiveness
- Impulsivity
- Distractibility
- Emotional liability
- Perceptual motor deficits
- Poor coordination, etc.
Finding relationships between a history of brain damage and these sorts of behavioral characteristics, Strauss argued that these behavioral markers could be used to infer the presence of brain damage in ambiguous cases, even when there was no documented evidence of neurological impairment.
Hyperactivity was given special status as the most valid indicator of brain damage.
Children were thought to display behavioral problems such as hyperactivity as a result of brain damage -- and -- children who were hyperactive were assumed to display brain damage because of the behaviors they displayed.
The circularity of this argument can be readily seen.
The use of the term "Minimal" in Minimal Brain Dysfunction, related to the assumption that degree of brain damage can exist along a continuum and that one that can have mild or minimal brain damage or dysfunction which is reflected primarily in its impact on the organization behavior, rather than in definitive neurological signs.
This notion of Minimal Brain Dysfunction flourished in the 1950's.
Corollaries of the MBD Concept
In addition to Strauss's role in the development of the concept of MBD, he also provided recommendations regarding the education of children with the disorder.
Strauss assumed children with this disorder to be over stimulated, due to their neurological difficulties which made it impossible to filter out extraneous stimuli.
This over stimulation was seen as contributing to the child's attention and activity-level problems.
As a result, Strauss suggested an educational environment where the child would be placed in small classes with the learning environment being specially designed to reduce excessive stimulation that could interfere with learning.
Behavioral difficulties of the MBD child were assumed to be helped by a "stimulus reduced" environment and made worse by an environment characterized by heightened levels of stimulation.
In the 1960's these sorts of educational suggestions were applied in the classroom by Cruikshank, without finding a great deal of support for the stimulus reduction hypothesis.
BEGINNINGS OF CHILD PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY
Developing interest in child psychopharmacology appeared in the late 1930's and the early 1940's when studies began to suggest that amphetamines were useful in reducing disruptive behavior and in improving academic performance.
Studies suggested that such medication helped at least half of the treated children.
MBD BECOMES THE HYPERACTIVE CHILD SYNDROME
By the early 1960's investigators began to seriously question the circular reasoning associated with the concept of MBD.
And, they began to question the notion of a unitary concept of brain damage which suggested a specific constellation of symptoms resulting from brain damage.
As a result, the focus on the concept of MBD diminished greatly, with this change in focus being accompanied by an increased interest in more specific problems such as learning disabilities, language disorders, mental retardation, and hyperactivity, rather than more general conditions such as the “Brain Damage Child”).
Consistent with this tendency to become more specific in focus, many clinicians came to focus on what, by this time, was becoming known as the Hyperactive Child Syndrome.
Chess (1960) , in a seminal article, argued that the primary defining feature of this disorder was an excessively heightened activity level.
"The hyperactive child is one who carries out activities at a higher than normal rate of speed than the average child or who is constantly in motion or both."
In this paper she stressed the need to consider objective evidence of the symptoms, apart from parent and teacher report, and to separate the Hyperactive Child Syndrome from the notion of the Brain Damaged Child.
While the focus was on hyperactivity, Stress noted that children with this disorder did often have an array of difficulties such as educational problems, oppositional behavior, peer problems, attentional difficulties, which could contribute to their difficulties.
The core symptom, however, was thought to be hyperactivity.
By the mid to late 1960's the focus of attention was clearly on hyperactive children (or hyperkinesis) rather than on those presumed to be brain damaged, minimally or otherwise.
Here it can be noted that in 1969 DSM II, published by the American Psychiatric Association, included the category HYPERKINETIC REACTION OF CHILDHOOD, which provided for a diagnosis of those children now referred to as ADHD.
Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood was seen has having:
- a relatively homogeneous set of symptoms, most notably excessive activity level.
- a relatively benign course and to often be outgrown by puberty (which we now know is not the case).
- Treatment was through stimulation medication and psychotherapy along with stimulus reduced educational environments.
THE DECADE OF THE 70'S - TRANSITION FROM HYPERKINESIS TO ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER
By the early to mid 1970's the concept of the hyperkinetic child syndrome was being broadened to include what investigators thought were associated characteristics such as impulsivity, low frustration tolerance, and attentiond difficulties.
While the focus of research interest had moved from a focus on brain damage to a focus on hyperactivity or hyperkinesis, this view began to change, in large part due to the work of McGill psychologist Virginia Douglas.
In 1972, Douglas, in her Presidential address to the Canadian Psychological Association, argued that deficits in sustained attention and impulse control were more likely to account for the difficulties seen in these children than just hyperactivity.
Here she cited findings from her research suggesting that hyperactive children show some of their greatest deficits on tasks like the Continuous Performance Test which assess characteristics such as vigilance, sustained attention and impulsivity.
She noted that a primary characteristic of this disorder was the extreme degree of variability in the task performance of such children on measures of attention.
She presented research to suggest that the degree of attentional control demonstrated by such children varied with reinforcement schedules (with attention being better under conditions of continuous reinforcement) and exceptionally poor under very thin schedules of partial reinforcement.
An additional argument for attentional problems being highlighted was that problems with attention and concentration seem to continue even into later life, while problems with activity level often decline as the child gets older.
Douglas's work on attentional processes, and the results of other research which stimulated by her work, appear to have been the primary reason for the later renaming of this disorder as Attention Deficit Disorder when the DSM III was published in 1980.
THE ROLE OF DIETARY FACTORS
In addition to the focus on attentional problems of hyperkinetic children, the 1970's witnessed much attention being directed toward the role of dietary factors as they relate to hyperkinetic behavior.
The assumption here was that allergic or toxic reactions to food additives such as dyes, preservatives, and salicylates caused hyperactive behavior.
This view, developed and popularized by Benjamin Feingold, claimed that over half of children with hyperactivity developed their problems because of diet related issues.
Suggested treatment was to involve buying or making foods that did not contain dyes, preservative or salicylates.
This focus on dietary factors as the cause of this disorder became so widespread that organized parent groups promoting the Feingold diet were organized in most states.
Despite the popularity, research designed to investigate the role of such dietary factors in the development or reduction of hyperactive behavior have generally not been supportive of this hypothesis.
The more recent view, that refined sugar is the culprit in hyperactivity, has also failed to receive significant empirical support.
While there may possibly be a very small number of children who's hyperactive behavior may be effected by some elements of their diet (and this is not even well documented), dietary factors are unlikely to be contributors to behavioral problems in children in the vast majority of instances.
STUDIES OF PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSIVITY IN HYPERACTIVE CHILDREN
An additional fruitful area of investigation in the 1970's involved studies to assess the pschophysiological responsiveness of hyperactive children.
These studies used measures such as GSR's, heart rate, EEG responses, and evoked potentials to assess hyperactive children.
Many of these studies were designed to test the notions of cortical over-stimulation, first advanced in the 1950's, that because children with MBD were not able to filter out stimuli they were over stimulated.
While many studies in this area were methodologically flawed, results of these investigations tended to provide more support for the notion that hyperactive children showed underreactive as opposed to overreactive electrophysiological responses to simulation.
They often tended to show lower amplitude responses to new stimulation and tended to habituate more rapidly to new stimulation than did normal children.
In some instances, this under reactivity or apparent underarousability to new stimuli appeared to be normalized by stimulant drugs.
This line of research seemed to argue against the notion of an over stimulated cerebral cortex as a cause of hyperactivity in children. In fact, it seems that the opposite may be the case; If anything, these children may be under aroused or underarousable in response to environmental stimulation.
By the mid-1970's such findings were cited as arguments for an "optimal stimulation view of hyperactivity" which holds that such children display less that optimal levels of stimulation and that their increased activity level and apparent distractibility can be seen as attempts to increase stimulation to some more optimal level.
THE 1980'S AND THE FORMAL ADVENT OF ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER (ADD)
In part, based on the research findings of Virgina Douglas, her colleagues, and those who had follow her research leads the focus in the late 1970's and early 1980's had shifted from hyperactivity to attention deficits.
Indeed, in 1980 when DSM III was published the disorder we are dealing with was renamed. Hyperkinetic Disorder of Childhood was replaced with Attention Deficit Disorder.
DEVELOPMENT OF DSM III
In 1980 the American Psychiatric Association published its Third Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.
Compared with the earlier diagnostic criteria for Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood (DSM II), the treatment of this category in DSM III was radically changed.
In DSM III the disorder was renamed Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD), so as to highlight the presumed central role of attentional difficulties (and to some extent impulsivity) in this condition.
Again, this change probably had a lot to do with research conducted by Virginia Douglas which highlighted the role of deficits in attention and impulse control in children with this disorder.
The treatment of this disorder in DSM III was noteworthy on several counts, including
- the renaming of the disorder
- the focus on inattention and impulsivity as defining features
- the development of more objective diagnostic criteria
- the presentation of numerical cutoff scores for symptoms
- age of onset criteria
- criteria for duration of condition
- exclusionary criteria for diagnosis
DSM III criteria, not only departed from those of DSM II, but also differed dramatically from ICD-9 criteria which had been published by the World Health Organization in 1978.
Here, ICD-9 emphasized “extreme and pervasive hyperactivity” as the defining features of the disorder rather than problem of inattention and impulsivity highlighted in DSM III.
Also notable in the DSM III criteria was the creation of ADD Subtypes.
Here, basic symptoms of the disorder were grouped into three classes.
- Symptoms of Inattention
- Symptoms of Impulsivity
- Symptoms of Hyperactivity
Based on the constellation of symptoms displayed, children could be diagnosed as having ADD, with or without hyperactivity.